Male brains are, on average, larger than females but this
doesn’t mean men are better at reading maps. Flickr/pedrosimoes7
The
latest neuroscience study of sex differences to hit the popular press has
inspired some familiar headlines. The Independent, for example, proclaims that:
The
hardwired difference between male and female brains could explain why men are
“better at map reading” (And why women are “better at remembering a
conversation”).
The study in question, published in PNAS, used a
technology called diffusion tensor imaging to model the structural
connectivity of the brains of nearly a thousand young people, ranging in age
from eight to 22.
It
reports greater connectivity within the hemispheres in males, but greater
connnectivity between the hemispheres in females. These findings, the authors
conclude in their scientific paper, suggest that male brains are structured to
facilitate connectivity between perception and coordinated action, whereas
female brains are designed to facilitate communication between analytical and
intuitive processing modes.
One
important possibility the authors don’t consider is that their results have
more to do with brain size than brain sex. Male brains are, on average, larger
than females and a large brain is not simply a smaller brain scaled up.
Larger
brains create different sorts of engineering problems and so – to minimise
energy demands, wiring costs, and communication times – there may physical reasons for different
arrangements in differently sized brains. The results may reflect the different wiring solutions of larger versus
smaller brains, rather than sex differences per se.
But
also, popular references to women’s brains being designed for social skills and
remembering conversations, or male brains for map reading, are utterly
misleading. Continue reading after the cut....
In
an larger earlier study (from which the participants
of the PNAS study were a subset), the same research team compellingly demonstrated
that the sex differences in the psychological skills they measured – executive
control, memory, reasoning, spatial processing, sensorimotor skills, and social
cognition – are almost all trivially small.
To
give a sense of the huge overlap in behaviour between males and females, of the
twenty-six possible comparisons, eleven sex differences were either non-existent,
or so small that if you were to select a boy and girl at random and compare
their scores on a task, the “right” sex would be superior less than 53% of the
time.
Even
the much-vaunted female advantage in social cognition, and male advantage in spatial
processing, was so modest that a randomly chosen boy would outscore a randomly
chosen girl on social cognition – and the girl would outscore the boy on
spatial processing – over 40% of the time.
As
for map-reading and remembering conversations, these weren’t measured at all.
Yet
the authors describe these differences as “pronounced” and as reflecting
“behavioural complementarity” – scientific jargon-speak for “men are from Mars,
women are from Venus”. Rather than drawing on their impressively rich data-set
to empirically test questions about how brain connectivity characteristics
relate to behaviour, the authors instead offer untested stereotype-based
speculation. Even though, with such considerable overlap in male/female
distributions, biological sex is a dismal guide to psychological ability.
Also
missing from the study is any mention of experience-dependent brain plasticity.
Why?
As
prominent feminist
neuroscientists have noted, the social phenomenon of gender means
that a person’s biological sex has a significant impact on the experiences
(including social, material, physical, and mental) she or he encounters which
will, in turn, leave neurological traces.
Yet
the researchers do not pay any attention to the gendered experiences (such as
hobbies, subjects studied at school or higher education, or participation in
sporting activities) of the young males and females in their sample.
This
absence has two consequences. First, the researchers miss an opportunity to
investigate whether gendered experiences might influence brain development and enhance the
acquisition of important skills valuable to all. The second consequence is
that, by failing to look at gendered social influences, the authors guarantee
that no data will be produced that challenge the notion of “hardwired”
male/female neural signatures.
These
characteristics of the PNAS study are very common in neuroscientific investigations of
male/female sex differences, and represent two important ways in which
scientific research can be subtly “neurosexist”, reinforcing and legitimating
gender stereotypes in ways that are not scientifically justified. And, when
researchers are “blinded” by sex, they can overlook potentially informative
research strategies.
Returning
to the popular representations, we can now see a striking disconnect with the
actual data. The research provides strong evidence for behavioural similarities
between the sexes. It provides no evidence that those modest behavioural sex
differences are associated with brain connectivity differences. And, it offers
no information about the developmental origins of either behavioural or brain
differences.
Yet,
the popular press presents it as evidence that “hardwired” sex differences
explain why men are from Mars and women are from Venus. While this is tediously
predictable, what is more surprising is for a study author to push along such
misinterpretations, claiming to have found evidence for “hardwired”
sex differences, and suggesting that this might explain behavioural sex
differences not actually measured in the study, such as in “intuition” skills
“linked with being good mothers”.
In
the latest issue of Trends in Cognitive Sciences, co-authors Rebecca
Jordan-Young, Anelis Kaiser and Gina Rippon and I argued that scientists investigating sex
differences have a responsibility to realise “how social assumptions influence
their research and, indeed, public understanding of it.” We then called on
scientists working in this area to: recognise
that there are important and exciting opportunities to change these social
assumptions through rigorous, reflective scientific inquiry and debate.
The
continuing importance of this message is only reinforced by this latest case
study in how easily scientific “neurosexism” can, with a little
stereotype-inspired imagination, contribute to inaccurate and harmful lay misunderstanding of what neuroscience
tells us about the sexes.
|
By: Cordelia Fine
No comments:
Post a Comment
Please add your comment...